Comparison with 1861 |
|
but some few cases both of
hybrids and mongrels long retaining uniformity of
character could be given. The variability, however, in the successive generations of mongrels is, perhaps, greater than in hybrids. |
|
This greater variability of
mongrels than of
hybrids does not seem to me
at all surprising. For the parents of mongrels are varieties, and mostly domestic varieties (very few experiments having been tried on natural varieties), and this implies in most cases
that there has been recent variability;
and therefore we might expect that such variability
would often continue and be super-added to
that arising from the mere
act of crossing. The slight degree
of variability
in hybrids from
the first cross or in the first
generation, in contrast with their extreme variability in
the succeeding generations, is a curious fact and deserves attention. For it bears on and corroborates
the view which I have taken on
the cause
of ordinary variability;
namely, that it is due to
the reproductive system being
eminently sensitive to any
change
in the
conditions of life, being thus often rendered either impotent or at least incapable of
its proper function of producing offspring identical with
the parent-form. Now hybrids in the first generation are descended from species (excluding those long
cultivated)
which have not had their reproductive systems in any way affected, and they are not variable; but hybrids themselves have their reproductive systems seriously affected, and their descendants are highly variable. |
|
But to return to our comparison of mongrels and hybrids: Gärtner states that mongrels are more liable than hybrids to revert to either parent-form; but this, if it be true, is certainly only a difference in degree. ↑1 blocks not present in 1859 1860 1861; present in 1866 1869 1872 | Moreover, Gärtner expressly states that hybrids from long-cultivated
plants are more subject to reversion than hybrids from species in their natural state; and this probably explains the singular difference in the results arrived at by different observers: thus, Max Wichura doubts whether hybrids ever revert to their parent- forms,
and he experimented on uncultivated species of willows; whilst Naudin, on the other hand, insists in the strongest terms on the almost universal tendency to reversion in hybrids, and he experimented chiefly on cultivated plants.
|
Gärtner further insists
that when any two species, although most closely allied to each other, are
|
but some few
cases both of cases both of 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
instances of both 1869 1872 |
hybrids and mongrels long retaining
uniformity of uniformity of 1859 1860 1861 1866 | a uniform 1869 1872 |
character could be given. The variability, however, in the successive generations of mongrels is, perhaps, greater than in hybrids. |
|
This greater variability
of of 1859 1860 1861 1866 | in 1869 1872 |
mongrels than
of of 1859 1860 1861 1866 | in 1869 1872 |
hybrids does not seem
to me to me 1859 1860 1861 1866 | to me 1869 1872 |
at all surprising. For the parents of mongrels are varieties, and mostly domestic varieties (very few experiments having been tried on natural varieties), and this implies
in most cases in most cases 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
OMIT 1869 1872 |
that there has been recent
variability; variability; 1859 1860 1861 1866 | variability, 1869 1872 |
and therefore we might expect that such variability and therefore we might expect that such variability 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
which 1869 1872 |
would often continue and
be super-added to be super-added to 1859 1861 |
be superadded to 1860 1866 |
be added to 1869 |
would augment 1872 |
that arising from the
mere mere 1859 1860 1861 1866 | mere 1869 1872 |
act of crossing. The slight
degree degree 1859 1860 1861 1866 | variability 1869 1872 |
of
variability variability 1859 1860 1861 1866 | hybrids 1869 1872 |
in
hybrids from hybrids from 1859 1860 1861 1866 | hybrids from 1869 1872 |
the first
cross or in the first cross or in the first 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
OMIT 1869 1872 |
generation, in contrast with
their extreme variability in their extreme variability in 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
that in 1872 |
OMIT 1869 |
the succeeding generations, is a curious fact and deserves attention. For it bears on
and corroborates and corroborates 1859 1860 1861 1866 | and corroborates 1869 1872 |
the view which I have taken
on on 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
of one of 1869 1872 |
the
cause cause 1859 1860 1861 1866 | causes 1869 1872 |
of ordinary
variability; variability; 1859 1860 1861 1866 1872 | variability: 1869 |
namely, that
it is due to it is due to 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
OMIT 1869 1872 |
the reproductive system
being being 1859 1860 1861 1866 | from being 1869 1872 |
eminently sensitive to
any any 1859 1860 1861 1866 | any 1869 1872 |
change change 1859 1860 1861 1866 | changed 1869 1872 |
in the in the 1859 1860 1861 1866 | in the 1869 1872 |
conditions of life,
being thus often rendered either impotent or at least incapable of being thus often rendered either impotent or at least incapable of 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
fails under these circumstances to perform 1869 1872 |
its proper function of producing offspring
identical with identical with 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
identical in all respects with 1869 |
closely similar in all respects to 1872 |
the parent-form. Now hybrids in the first generation are descended from species (excluding those
long long 1859 1860 1861 1866 1872 | long 1869 |
cultivated) cultivated) 1859 1860 1861 1866 1872 | long-cultivated) 1869 |
which have not had their reproductive systems in any way affected, and they are not variable; but hybrids themselves have their reproductive systems seriously affected, and their descendants are highly variable. |
|
But to return to our comparison of mongrels and hybrids: Gärtner states that mongrels are more liable than hybrids to revert to either parent-form; but this, if it be true, is certainly only a difference in degree. ↑1 blocks not present in 1859 1860 1861; present in 1866 1869 1872 | Moreover, Gärtner expressly states that hybrids from long-cultivated
plants are more subject to reversion than hybrids from species in their natural state; and this probably explains the singular difference in the results arrived at by different observers: thus, Max Wichura doubts whether hybrids ever revert to their parent- forms,
and he experimented on uncultivated species of willows; whilst Naudin, on the other hand, insists in the strongest terms on the almost universal tendency to reversion in hybrids, and he experimented chiefly on cultivated plants.
|
Gärtner further
insists insists 1859 1860 1861 | states 1866 1869 1872 |
that when any two species, although most closely allied to each other, are
|