Comparison with 1861 |
|
diagram would still hold good;
and,
on
the principle of inheritance, all the forms descended
from A, or from I,
would have something in common. In a tree we can specify
this or that branch, though at the actual fork the two unite and blend together. We could not, as I have said, define the several groups; but we could pick out types, or forms, representing most of the characters of each group, whether large or small, and thus give a general idea of the value of the differences between them. This is what we should be driven to, if we were ever to succeed in collecting all the forms in any class
which have lived throughout all time and space. We shall certainly
never succeed in making so perfect a collection: nevertheless, in certain classes, we are tending in
this direction;
and Milne Edwards has lately insisted, in an able paper, on the high importance of looking to types, whether or not we can separate and define the groups to which such types belong. |
|
Finally, we have seen that natural selection, which results
from the struggle for existence, and which almost inevitably induces
extinction and divergence of character in the many descendants from
one dominant
parent-species, explains that great and universal feature in the affinities of all organic beings, namely, their
subordination subordination 1859 1860 1861 1869 1872 | sub-ordination 1866 |
in group under group. We use the element of descent in classing the individuals of both sexes and of all ages,
although having few characters in common, although having few characters in common, 1859 1860 1861 |
although having but few characters in common, 1866 |
OMIT 1869 1872 |
under one species;
we use descent in classing acknowledged varieties, however different
they may be
from their parent;
and I believe this
element of descent is the hidden bond of connexion which naturalists have sought under the term of the Natural System. On this idea of the natural system being, in so far as it has been perfected, genealogical in its arrangement, with the grades of difference between the descendants from a common parent,
|
diagram would still hold
good; good; 1859 1860 1861 1866 | good 1869 1872 |
and, and, 1859 1860 1861 1866 | and 1869 1872 |
on on 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
would be natural; for, on 1869 1872 |
the principle of inheritance, all the forms
descended descended 1859 1860 1861 1866 | descended, 1869 1872 |
from A, or from I, from A, or from I, 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
for instance, from A, 1869 1872 |
would have something in common. In a tree we can
specify specify 1859 1860 1861 1866 | distinguish 1869 1872 |
this or that branch, though at the actual fork the two unite and blend together. We could not, as I have said, define the several groups; but we could pick out types, or forms, representing most of the characters of each group, whether large or small, and thus give a general idea of the value of the differences between them. This is what we should be driven to, if we were ever to succeed in collecting all the forms in any
class class 1859 1860 1861 1866 | one class 1869 1872 |
which have lived throughout all time and space.
We shall assuredly We shall assuredly 1861 1866 |
We shall certainly 1859 1860 |
Assuredly we shall 1869 1872 |
never succeed in making so perfect a collection: nevertheless, in certain classes, we are tending
in in 1859 1860 1861 1866 | towards 1869 1872 |
this
direction; direction; 1859 1860 1861 1866 | end; 1869 1872 |
and Milne Edwards has lately insisted, in an able paper, on the high importance of looking to types, whether or not we can separate and define the groups to which such types belong. |
|
Finally, we have seen that natural selection, which
results results 1859 1860 1861 1866 1869 | follows 1872 |
from the struggle for existence, and which almost inevitably
induces induces 1859 1860 1861 1866 | leads to 1869 1872 |
extinction and divergence of character in the
many descendants from many descendants from 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
descendants from 1869 |
descendants from any 1872 |
one
dominant dominant 1859 1860 1861 1866 1869 | dominant 1872 |
parent-species, explains that great and universal feature in the affinities of all organic beings, namely, their
sub-ordination sub-ordination 1866 | subordination 1859 1860 1861 1869 1872 |
in group under group. We use the element of descent in classing the individuals of both sexes and of all
ages, ages, 1859 1860 1861 1866 | ages 1869 1872 |
although having but few characters in common, although having but few characters in common, 1866 |
although having few characters in common, 1859 1860 1861 |
OMIT 1869 1872 |
under one
species; species; 1859 1860 1861 1866 | species, 1869 1872 |
we use descent in classing acknowledged varieties, however different we use descent in classing acknowledged varieties, however different 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
although 1869 1872 |
they may
be be 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
have but few characters in common; we use descent in classing acknowledged varieties, however different they may be 1869 1872 |
from their
parent; parent; 1859 1860 1861 1866 1869 | parents; 1872 |
and I believe
this this 1859 1860 1861 1866 1869 | that this 1872 |
element of descent is the hidden bond of connexion which naturalists have sought under the term of the Natural System. On this idea of the natural system being, in so far as it has been perfected, genealogical in its arrangement, with the grades of difference
between the descendants from a common parent, between the descendants from a common parent, 1859 1860 1861 1866 |
OMIT 1869 1872 |
|