Comparison with 1861 |
|
Text in this page (from subtitle, paragraph 3810, word 1 to subtitle, paragraph 3810, word 15) is not present in 1861 |
It has been objected to the foregoing view
on
the origin of
instinct instinct 1861 | instincts 1866 1869 1872 |
that "the variations of structure and of instinct must have been simultaneous and accurately adjusted to each other, as a modification in the one without an immediate corresponding change in the other would have been fatal." The force of this objection
seems seems 1861 1866 | rests 1869 1872 |
entirely
to rest to rest 1861 1866 | to rest 1869 1872 |
on the assumption that the changes in both
instinct
and structure are abrupt. To take as an illustration the case of the larger titmouse (Parus major) alluded to in the last
chapter: this bird often holds the seeds of the yew between its feet on a branch, and hammers away
till it gets into
the kernel. Now what special difficulty would there be in natural selection preserving
each slight variation of beak, each slight variation of beak, 1861 1866 |
all the slight individual variations in the shape of the beak, which were 1869 1872 |
better and better adapted to break open
seeds, seeds, 1861 1866 | the seeds, 1869 1872 |
until a beak was formed, as well constructed for this purpose as that of the nuthatch, at the same time that
hereditary hereditary 1861 1866 | hereditary 1869 1872 |
habit, or
compulsion compulsion 1861 1866 | compulsion, 1869 1872 |
from the want of other food, or the preservation of chance from the want of other food, or the preservation of chance 1861 1866 |
or spontaneous 1869 1872 |
variations of taste,
made made 1861 1866 | led 1869 1872 |
the bird
more more 1861 1866 |
to become more 1869 1872 |
and more of a seed-eater? In this case the beak is supposed to be slowly modified by natural selection, subsequently to, but in accordance with, slowly changing
habit; habit; 1861 1866 | habits 1869 1872 |
but but 1861 1866 |
or taste; but 1869 1872 |
let the feet of the titmouse vary and grow larger from correlation with the beak, or from any other unknown cause, and
is
it
very very 1861 1866 | is not 1869 1872 |
improbable that such larger feet
might might 1861 | would 1866 1869 1872 |
lead the bird to climb more and more
until it acquired
even even 1861 1866 | even 1869 1872 |
the remarkable climbing instinct and
capacity capacity 1861 1866 | power 1869 1872 |
of the
nuthatch? nuthatch? 1861 1866 | nuthatch. 1869 1872 |
In this case a gradual change of structure is supposed to lead to changed instinctive
habits habits 1861 | habits. 1866 1869 1872 |
of
life. life. 1861 | life. 1866 1869 1872 |
To take one more case: few instincts are more remarkable than that which leads the swift of the Eastern Islands to make its nest wholly of inspissated saliva.
|
Objections
to
the
Theory
of
Natural
Selection
as
applied
to
Instincts:
Neuter
and
Sterile
Insects
.
Insects
. 1866 1869 |
Insects
. 1872 |
|
It has been objected to the foregoing view
of
the origin of
instincts instincts 1866 1869 1872 | instinct 1861 |
that "the variations of structure and of instinct must have been simultaneous and accurately adjusted to each other, as a modification in the one without an immediate corresponding change in the other would have been fatal." The force of this objection
rests rests 1869 1872 | seems 1861 1866 |
entirely
....... 1869 1872 | to rest 1861 1866 |
on the assumption that the changes in
both both 1861 1866 1869 | the 1872 |
instinct instinct 1861 1866 1869 | instincts 1872 |
and structure are abrupt. To take as an illustration the case of the larger titmouse (Parus major) alluded to in
the last the last 1861 1866 1869 | a previous 1872 |
chapter: this bird often holds the seeds of the yew between its feet on a branch, and hammers
away away 1861 1866 1869 |
with its beak 1872 |
till it gets
into into 1861 1866 1869 | at 1872 |
the kernel. Now what special difficulty would there be in natural selection preserving
all the slight individual variations in the shape of the beak, which were all the slight individual variations in the shape of the beak, which were 1869 1872 |
each slight variation of beak, 1861 1866 |
better and better adapted to break open
the seeds, the seeds, 1869 1872 | seeds, 1861 1866 |
until a beak was formed, as well constructed for this purpose as that of the nuthatch, at the same time that
....... 1869 1872 | hereditary 1861 1866 |
habit, or
compulsion, compulsion, 1869 1872 | compulsion 1861 1866 |
or spontaneous or spontaneous 1869 1872 |
from the want of other food, or the preservation of chance 1861 1866 |
variations of taste,
led led 1869 1872 | made 1861 1866 |
the bird
to become more to become more 1869 1872 |
more 1861 1866 |
and more of a seed-eater? In this case the beak is supposed to be slowly modified by natural selection, subsequently to, but in accordance with, slowly changing
habits habits 1869 1872 | habit; 1861 1866 |
or taste; but or taste; but 1869 1872 |
but 1861 1866 |
let the feet of the titmouse vary and grow larger from correlation with the beak, or from any other unknown cause, and
....... 1869 1872 | is 1861 1866 |
it
is not is not 1869 1872 | very 1861 1866 |
improbable that such larger feet
would would 1866 1869 1872 | might 1861 |
lead the bird to climb more
and more and more 1861 1869 1872 | and more 1866 |
until it acquired
....... 1869 1872 | even 1861 1866 |
the remarkable climbing instinct and
power power 1869 1872 | capacity 1861 1866 |
of the
nuthatch. nuthatch. 1869 1872 | nuthatch? 1861 1866 |
In this case a gradual change of structure is supposed to lead to changed instinctive
habits. habits. 1866 1869 1872 | habits 1861 |
....... 1866 1869 1872 | of 1861 |
....... 1866 1869 1872 | life. 1861 |
To take one more case: few instincts are more remarkable than that which leads the swift of the Eastern Islands to make its nest wholly of inspissated saliva.
|